Revision as of 02:12, 9 June 2024 by Bot (Created page with "<div class="d-none"><math> \newcommand{\NA}{{\rm NA}} \newcommand{\mat}[1]{{\bf#1}} \newcommand{\exref}[1]{\ref{##1}} \newcommand{\secstoprocess}{\all} \newcommand{\NA}{{\rm NA}} \newcommand{\mathds}{\mathbb}</math></div> Tversky and Kahneman<ref group="Notes" >K. McKean, “Decisions, Decisions,” pp. 22--31.</ref> asked a group of subjects to carry out the following task. They are told that: <blockquote> Linda is 31, single, outspoken, and very bright. She maj...")
(diff) ← Older revision | Latest revision (diff) | Newer revision → (diff)
BBy Bot
Jun 09'24

Exercise

[math] \newcommand{\NA}{{\rm NA}} \newcommand{\mat}[1]{{\bf#1}} \newcommand{\exref}[1]{\ref{##1}} \newcommand{\secstoprocess}{\all} \newcommand{\NA}{{\rm NA}} \newcommand{\mathds}{\mathbb}[/math]

Tversky and Kahneman[Notes 1] asked a group of subjects to carry out the

following task. They are told that:

Linda is 31, single, outspoken, and very bright. She majored in philosophy in college. As a student, she was deeply concerned with racial discrimination and other social issues, and participated in anti-nuclear demonstrations.

The subjects are then asked to rank the likelihood of various alternatives, such as:\hfill\break \indent(1) Linda is active in the feminist movement.\hfill\break \indent(2) Linda is a bank teller.\hfill\break \indent(3) Linda is a bank teller and active in the feminist movement.\hfill\break Tversky and Kahneman found that between 85 and 90 percent of the subjects rated alternative (1) most likely, but alternative (3) more likely than alternative (2). Is it? They call this phenomenon the conjunction fallacy, and note that it appears to be unaffected by prior training in probability or statistics. Is this phenomenon a fallacy? If so, why? Can you give a possible explanation for the subjects' choices?

Notes

  1. K. McKean, “Decisions, Decisions,” pp. 22--31.